Open Access
Subscription Access
The More the Merrier:Dogs can Assess Quantities in Food-Choice Tasks
Animals in their natural environment often face situa-tions where it may be advantageous for them to be able to make decisions based on numerical or quantity discrimination. Canids like pet dogs, wolves and coy-otes have been known to have a preliminary sense of number. We tested 303 unique free-ranging dogs for seven food-choice tasks, skewed in terms of stimulus: olfactory, visual and reward obtained. The dogs pri-marily used olfactory cues in the decision-making process, rather than visual cues, to discriminate between different quantities in a context-dependent manner.
Keywords
Food-Choice Task, Free-Ranging Dogs, Numerical Cognition, Quantity Discrimination, Stimulus.
User
Font Size
Information
- Gelman, R. and Cordes, S., Counting in animals and humans. In Language, Brain, and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler, The MIT Press, 2001, pp. 279–301.
- Merritt, D. J., DeWind, N. K. and Brannon, E. M., Comparative cognition of number representation. Oxf. Handb. Comp. Cogn., 2012; doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195392661.013.0024.
- Gordon, P., Numerical cognition without words: evidence from Amazonia. Science, 2004, 306, 496–499.
- Beran, M. J. and Rumbaugh, D. M., Enumeration by chimpanzees on a computerized task. Anim. Cogn., 2001, 4, 81–89.
- Beran, M. J. and Beran, M. M., Chimpanzees remember the results of one-by-one addition of food items to sets over extended time periods. Psychol. Sci., 2004, 15, 94–99.
- Evans, T. A., Beran, M. J., Harris, E. H. and Rice, D. F., Quantity judgments of sequentially presented food items by capuchin mon-keys (Cebus apella). Anim. Cogn., 2009, 12, 97–105.
- Agrillo, C., Piffer, L. and Bisazza, A., Number versus continuous quantity in numerosity judgments by fish. Cognition, 2011, 119, 281–287.
- Lucon-Xiccato, T., Gatto, E. and Bisazza, A., Quantity discrimina-tion by treefrogs. Anim. Behav., 2018, 139, 61–69.
- Vonk, J. and Beran, M. J., Bears ‘count’ too: quantity estimation and comparison in black bears, Ursus americanus. Anim. Behav., 2012, 84, 231–238.
- Perdue, B. M., Talbot, C. F., Stone, A. M. and Beran, M. J., Put-ting the elephant back in the herd: elephant relative quantity judgments match those of other species. Anim. Cogn., 2012, 15, 955–961.
- Dacke, M. and Srinivasan, M. V., Evidence for counting in insects. Anim. Cogn., 2008, 11, 683–689.
- Pepperberg, I. M., Cognitive and communicative abilities of Grey parrots. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 2006, 100, 77–86.
- Ward, C. and Smuts, B. B., Quantity-based judgments in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris). Anim. Cogn., 2006, 10, 71–80.
- West, R. E. and Young, R. J., Do domestic dogs show any evi-dence of being able to count? Anim. Cogn., 2002, 5, 183–186.
- Utrata, E., Virányi, Z. and Range, F., Quantity discrimination in wolves (Canis lupus). Front. Psychol., 2012, 3, 505.
- Baker, J. M., Shivik, J. and Jordan, K. E., Tracking of food quanti-ty by coyotes (Canis latrans). Behav. Process., 2011, 88, 72–75.
- Range, F., Jenikejew, J., Schröder, I. and Virányi, Z., Difference in quantity discrimination in dogs and wolves. Front. Psychol., 2014, 5, 1299.
- Lampe, M., Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J. and Virányi, Z., The effects of domestication and ontogeny on cognition in dogs and wolves. Sci. Rep., 2017, 7, 11690.
- Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C. and Tomasello, M., The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science, 2002, 298, 1634–1636.
- Weber, M. and Schneider, L., Marginal utility theory and the fun-damental law of psychophysics. Soc. Sci. Q., 1975, 56, 21–36.
- Macpherson, K. and Roberts, W. A., Can dogs count? Learn. Motiv., 2013, 44, 241–251.
- Bhadra, A. et al., The meat of the matter: a rule of thumb for scavenging dogs? Ethol. Ecol. Evol., 2016, 28, 427–440.
- Team R. Core. R: A language and environment for statistical com-puting, 2003, p. 201.
- Davis, H. and Pérusse, R., Numerical competence in animals: definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda. Behav. Brain Sci., 1988, 11, 561.
- Paul, M., Sen Majumder, S., Nandi, A. K. and Bhadra, A., Selfish mothers indeed! Resource-dependent conflict over extended parental care in free-ranging dogs. Open Sci., 2015, 2, 150580.
- Bhadra, A. and Bhadra, A., Preference for meat is not innate in dogs. J. Ethol., 2014, 32, 15–22.
- Darwin, C., The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex, D. Appleton, 1896, vol. 1.
- Shettleworth, S. J., Clever animals and killjoy explanations in comparative psychology. Trends Cogn. Sci., 2010, 14, 477– 481.
- Thorndike, E. L., 1911. Edward Lee Thorndike. Anim. Intell., 1874, 1949.
- Thomas, R. K., Investigating cognitive abilities in animals: unreal-ized potential. Cogn. Brain Res., 1996, 3, 157–166.
- Zentall, T. R., Wasserman, E. A., Lazareva, O. F., Thompson, R. K. R. and Rattermann, M. J., Concept learning in animals. Comp. Cogn. Behav. Rev., 2008, 3, 13–45.
- Gallistel, C. R. and Gelman, R., Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers. Trends Cogn. Sci., 2000, 4, 59–65.
- Harrington, F. H. and Mech, L. D., Wolf howling and its role in territory maintenance. Behaviour, 1979, 68, 207–249.
- Sillero-Zubiri, C. and Macdonald, D. W., Scent-marking and terri-torial behaviour of Ethiopian wolves Canis simensis. J. Zool., 1998, 245, 351–361.
Abstract Views: 359
PDF Views: 119